Back on topic, well close, I think the term tone-deaf is mis-used. I know of people who find difficulty singing anywhere near in tune but when listening to music will analyse the sound very critically - maybe ton-dumb would be more appropriate in this situation.
What is the aural equivalent of colour blindness?
Collapse
X
-
Originally posted by Gordon View Post...Without sight someone like the Guardian example or Dawkins' bat rely only on sound so I can't see how the echolocation process can find the "colour" or even apply a textural equivalent in sound, it's a property that does not belong in the sound world. There is no harm in a bat applying the label "colour" to some property of an object but it is not the same thing as light...
It's a lot easier to imagine this happening in a world where evolution has produced creatures attuned to it - which is why bats or whales are good examples. Single examples, such as the chap in the Guardian, don't represent an evolutionary trend - he has adapted remarkably by using different senses, but that's not evolutionary change. Our sensitivity to colour is a result of evolution, however. Presumably colour definition was important to our ancestors (all primates have good colour recognition - useful perhaps for creatures that lived in the low light levels of forests and ate plenty of fruit).
Comment
-
-
Nevalti
Originally posted by Gordon View PostI suspect that part of this apparent variability in the ability to hear small differences in sound stimuli is down to the individual.
The parallel with colour blindness though is inverted; the sight defect is rare whereas the rarity in HiFi enthusiasts seems to be an enhanced sensitivity, not a defect.
Comment
-
Originally posted by Pabmusic View PostUnderstandable, of course, until you realise that the labels we use for colour - red, blue, yellow - are not qualities of specific wavelengths of light and cannot be tied exclusively to the electro-magnetic spectrum. They are constructs of the brain for use in modelling the virtual world that is actually what we see. (This is all very counter-intuitive.) They help us make sense of the light we see. That being so, those qualia (labels) could just as easily be applied to other qualities of our virtual world, such as touch or echolocation. So that a person who has never seen anything may nevertheless construct a virtual model of reality using qualia that (if only we could experience them) are 'red' or 'blue', because they help make sense of his or her virtual world.
It's a lot easier to imagine this happening in a world where evolution has produced creatures attuned to it - which is why bats or whales are good examples. Single examples, such as the chap in the Guardian, don't represent an evolutionary trend - he has adapted remarkably by using different senses, but that's not evolutionary change. Our sensitivity to colour is a result of evolution, however. Presumably colour definition was important to our ancestors (all primates have good colour recognition - useful perhaps for creatures that lived in the low light levels of forests and ate plenty of fruit).
Coming back to sound we can ask a similar question; a keen auditory sense is valuable for survival, both in terms of low levels of sound - hearing a threat or food on the hoof at a distance, perhaps unseen - but also the analysis of it to extract valuable additional information about the environment. The physiology of the ear is strange, being made of a bank of resonators the excitation of which has a time/frequency characteristic leading to a similar sort of effect to the blind spot we all have. Whatever processing the brain does it is of the sensory input from these resonators which have limited ability. Excite the ear with a sound and it becomes unable to hear closely related sound that accompanies the first if it is at a lower level - the known masking phenomenon. What was the evolutionary value of that? Loud sound is more important than soft when danger threatens?
Interesting that we have two of most things including eyes and ears; without two of those our spatial sense would be degraded. The spacing and placement of our eyes allows 3D/binocular vision [some animals, fish and birds don't have this placement of course] but our ears are separated.
Comment
-
-
Originally posted by Gordon View PostIsn't that what I said above? Or am I being slow?...
Originally posted by Gordon View PostAgreed, but I'm nor sure about the low light bit - human vision degrades to monochrome in low light. Once creatures became sensitive to light in the first place [plants are too] then colour clearly had an evolutionary benefit and that is very old. Apart from seeing your enemies, environmental hazards etc or your food supply, perhaps colour added a valuable refinement. However, when did the sensation become not only a practical advantage but also an aesthetic phenomenon? What evolutionary value does an aesthetic sense have? Reaches for Dawkins, he must have something to say about it...
Much too simplistic of course, but you see what I was getting at?
Originally posted by Gordon View PostInteresting that we have two of most things including eyes and ears; without two of those our spatial sense would be degraded. The spacing and placement of our eyes allows 3D/binocular vision [some animals, fish and birds don't have this placement of course] but our ears are separated.
Comment
-
-
#12 antongould, that Flanders and Swann song is called 'Slow Train'. I have it on the LP 'At the drop of another hat'. It isnt on my CD 'At the drop of a hat', but it must be out there somewhere.
Marvellous pair of comics. I'm particularly fond of 'Madeira M'Dear': " ... now if it were gin you'd be wrong to say yes, the evil gin does would be hard to assess; besides, its inclined to affect me prowess, so have some madeira m'dear! ..."
Comment
-
-
Originally posted by MarkG View Post
However, he could not sing in tune. I seem to have inherited the some of the same issues. In particular, after you have your father pointing out the record going up and down in pitch every time there is one on the radio you learn to notice it. I never had the sensitivity to tape flutter (he could not explain what he was hearing, but we went to buy a tape recorder and he instantly rejected a Ferrograph and a Revox, and chose a Simon SP5 which turned out to have much lower flutter than the others.)
So now, if I sing I can instantly hear that I am out of tune, and which way I am wrong. But by the time I have hit the note I wanted I am three bars behind. Result, I don't even hum to myself (or at least not loud enough that I can hear it.) Maybe some of this really is inherited rather than learnt.
Comment
-
-
Originally posted by MarkG View Post
Comment
-
-
Nevalti
Originally posted by MarkG View Post
My experience is very similar to OldTechie and his father. I didn't realise that some people couldn't tell if it was tape, LP or live. It usually seemed glaringly obvious to me. These days, with good digital recordings, it is rather more difficult but I can usually tell if it is live or recorded by the absence of low level information which has been discarded as 'noise' during the recording process. On DAB transmissions they throw out a vast amount more so that it doesn't sound live even when it is.
As for singing - I can't quite make a complete octave these days so my repertoire is severely limited but I can hit the right notes within my limited range. Rather oddly, I often have difficulty joining in with other singers on the right note but when I see the oboist ready to play the tuning 'A' at a concert, I sometimes hum that note before he plays it. I am often spot on and usually within a semi-tone.
Comment
-
Originally posted by Nevalti View PostRather oddly, I often have difficulty joining in with other singers on the right note but when I see the oboist ready to play the tuning 'A' at a concert, I sometimes hum that note before he plays it. I am often spot on and usually within a semi-tone.[FONT=Comic Sans MS][I][B]Numquam Satis![/B][/I][/FONT]
Comment
-
Comment