Originally posted by ferneyhoughgeliebte
View Post
In the excellent aural training classes that I attended at Royal College of Music millions of years ago it was not unusual to have to sight-sing passages from the Cantatas of Webern and I'll never forget a brief passage in (I think) the second one where a four part chord D-G-B-E (with D as the bass note) is followed by another C#-F#-A#-D# by the upper two parts moving upwards and the lower two downwards by a semitone; I incurred the displeasure of the person taking the class when I noted the first to be an added sixth major chord in second inversion, revealing that I had heard the passage "tonally" - "you're not supposed to hear it like that!". This was, for me, the sole blot on the landscape of those memorable classes; I didn't pursue it but couldn't help but think to myself "did Webern really tell his listeners how to hear - and how not to hear - any of his music?". Clearly, he "alwys composed with intervals" too; which of us doesn't, in one way or another? The ear finds what it finds - and another example of this (albeit unrelated to "tonality" issues) is whenever I hear, for example, Elgar's First Symphony or Schönberg's D minor String Quartet, I invariably notice things that I'd not noticed on previous hearings because my attention had been taken more by other things in those works; OK, this kind of thing is likely to happen only when the work has so very much to offer and is so concentrated, but its relevance here is to the way in which the listener hears what he/she does in any piece and, clearly, no two listeners will experience identical responses or find the same things in the same proportions of importance and impact.
Originally posted by ferneyhoughgeliebte
View Post
Comment